There is a world somewhere between reality and fiction. Although ignored by many, it is very real and so are those living in it. This forum is about the natural world. Here, wild animals will be heard and respected. The forum offers a glimpse into an unknown world as well as a room with a view on the present and the future. Anyone able to speak on behalf of those living in the emerald forest and the deep blue sea is invited to join.
--- Peter Broekhuijsen ---

  • 0 Vote(s) - 0 Average
  • 1
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
  • 5
Cheetah Predation

Sanju Offline
Senior member
*****
#16

(07-07-2020, 08:25 AM)scilover Wrote: It looks like the cheetah ate its kill already and was full. Look at its belly. I guess it didn't bothered about the warthogs eating its leftovers.
True.
Cheetahs don't show interest in conflicts (including intra-specific) and fights with other animals because it may cost deadly to their fragile frame work built for speed.
Reply

cheetah Offline
Banned
#17

https://trackingthewild.com/can-a-cheeta...n-ostrich/
1 user Likes cheetah's post
Reply

Canada Acinonyx sp. Offline
Cheetah Enthusiast
***
#18

Cheetah predation on giraffe calves


   
4 users Like Acinonyx sp.'s post
Reply

Oman Lycaon Offline
أسد الأطلس
*****
Moderators
#19

Sudan cheetah with chital kill.

" target="_blank" class="post_link">
3 users Like Lycaon's post
Reply

Canada Acinonyx sp. Offline
Cheetah Enthusiast
***
#20

Cheetah with young zebra kill


*This image is copyright of its original author
Reply

Canada Acinonyx sp. Offline
Cheetah Enthusiast
***
#21

Young cheetah learning to kill


*This image is copyright of its original author
Reply

Canada Acinonyx sp. Offline
Cheetah Enthusiast
***
#22

Cheetah predation in Egypt from Saleh 2001


*This image is copyright of its original author
1 user Likes Acinonyx sp.'s post
Reply

Canada Acinonyx sp. Offline
Cheetah Enthusiast
***
#23

Hayward, M. W., Hofmeyr, M., O’Brien, J., & Kerley, G. I. H. (2006). Prey preferences of the cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) (Felidae: Carnivora): morphological limitations or the need to capture rapidly consumable prey before kleptoparasites arrive? Journal of Zoology
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00184.x 
https://sci-hub.se/https://zslpublications.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00184.x



Prey preferences of the cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) (Felidae: Carnivora): morphological limitations or the need to capture rapidly consumable prey before kleptoparasites arrive?

Abstract 
As a charismatic carnivore that is vulnerable to extinction, many studies have been conducted on predation by the cheetah Acinonyx jubatus. Cheetah are generally considered to capture medium-sized prey; however, which species are actually preferred and why has yet to be addressed. We used data from 21 published and two unpublished studies from six countries throughout the distribution of the cheetah to determine which prey species were preferred and which were avoided using Jacobs’ index. The mean Jacobs’ index value for each prey species was used as the dependent variable in multiple regression, with prey abundance and prey body mass as predictive variables. Cheetah prefer to kill and actually kill the most available prey present at a site within a body mass range of 23–56 kg with a peak (mode) at 36 kg. Blesbok, impala, Thomson’s and Grant’s gazelles, and springbok are significantly preferred, whereas prey outside this range are generally avoided. The morphological adaptations of the cheetah appear to have evolved to capture medium-sized prey that can be subdued with minimal risk of injury. Coincidentally, these species can be consumed rapidly before kleptoparasites arrive. These results are discussed through the premise of optimality theory whereby decisions made by the predator maximize the net energetic benefits of foraging. Information is also presented that allows conservation managers to determine which prey species should be in adequate numbers at cheetah reintroduction sites to support a cheetah population. Conversely, these results will illustrate which potential prey species of local conservation concern should be monitored for impact from cheetahs as several species are likely to be preyed upon more frequently than others.

Introduction 
The cheetah Acinonyx jubatus is a highly specialized, cursorial felid that has evolved as a rapid pursuit specialist (Mills & Harvey, 2001). It once occurred throughout much of Africa, South Asia and the Middle East (Nowell & Jackson, 1996). This distribution has declined (Nowell & Jackson, 1996), and today the cheetah is listed as vulnerable (Cat Specialist Group 2002, 2004), largely through habitat loss (Marker, 1998; Frame, 1999). As the fastest living land mammal, the cheetah is capable of speeds up to 104–112 km h1 for short distances (Sharp, 1997; Mills & Harvey, 2001). This is possible through its highly flexible spine; hardened, grooved and pointed foot pads; shortened canine teeth that allow increased airflow; and semi-retractable claws (Eaton, 1974; Frame, 1999; Russell & Bryant, 2001). Despite these morphological adaptations, the wide distribution of the cheetah suggests it is capable of killing a wide range of prey species. Reported prey range in mass from 2 kg hares Lepus sp. and warthog Phacochoerus africanus piglets to adult wildebeest Connochaetes taurinus and zebra Equus burchelli weighing up to 270 kg (Eaton, 1974) and even giraffe and buffalo calves (Pienaar, 1969). Adult cheetahs weigh between 30 and 72 kg (Stuart & Stuart, 2000), which means they need to prey upon large vertebrates (445% of their own body mass) to satisfy their energetic demands (Carbone et al., 1999). Their relatively fragile teeth, skull and jaw musculature largely restricts cheetahs to feeding on flesh (van Valkenburgh, Teaford & Walker, 1990). Approximately half of all cheetah pursuits end in kills (Frame, 1999). This high hunting success rate is thought to reflect a highly predictable outcome and the very highenergy expenditure during hunts that penalizes unsuccessful attempts (Bertram, 1979). Accurate prediction of the outcome of hunts may arise because cheetahs select less vigilant individuals from the herds they are hunting (Fitzgibbon, 1989). Hunting group sizes vary from solitary individuals, to females with cubs and coalitions of territorial males (Caro & Collins, 1986; Caro, 1994), with larger groups having a higher hunting success rate (Eaton, 1974) and taking larger prey (Caro, 1994). Hunting cheetah groups in Etosha exhibited coordination between members (Stander, 1990), although Caro (1994) rejects the evidence of cooperative hunting. Cheetahs lose up to 12% of kills to kleptoparasites, particularly lion Panthera leo and spotted hyaena Crocuta crocuta (Kruuk, 1972; Schaller, 1972), and to reduce encounters with these competitors it is largely diurnal (Schaller, 1968; Stander, 1990). As it is competitively inferior to virtually all other large predators, even vultures (Frame, 1999), the cheetah only occasionally scavenges (Schaller, 1972) the 2.8 kg of meat it requires daily (Frame, 1999) and bolts down its food before competitors arrive (Kruuk & Turner, 1967). Cheetahs inhabiting areas of denser vegetation suffer lower levels of kleptoparasitism than conspecifics in more open areas (Mills, Broomhall & du Toit, 2004). In this study, we aimed to use dietary and prey abundance data collected from various studies conducted throughout the distribution of the cheetah to determine which, if any, prey species the cheetah prefers and those it avoids. Further, we attempt to explain why particular species are preferred or avoided using various ecological features of the prey and based on the premise of optimality (Pyke, Pulliam & Charnov, 1977; Krebs, 1978). We also investigated whether denser habitats afford cheetah refuge from kleptoparasitism and thereby make it worthwhile to capture larger prey than more open areas where the likelihood of kleptoparasitism is increased. Our analyses have followed that of Hayward & Kerley (2005) to allow direct comparison between the causes of prey preferences of lion and cheetah and, subsequently, the rest of Africa’s large predatory guild.

Methods 
Data collection 
Nineteen published studies and two theses found in the literature were combined with two unpublished studies that described the diet of the cheetah along with some measure of prey abundance (either actual or relative; Table 1). Several of these studies were conducted over a long term and these allowed temporally separated prey preferences to be calculated as prey abundance changed over time (Table 1). Others provided such information from different study regions


*This image is copyright of its original author

Table 1). Such partitioning has been used previously in studies of carnivore ecology (see Creel & Creel, 2002). Consequently, a total of 28 assessments of prey preference were calculated from sites throughout the distributional range of the cheetah (Table 1). We do not believe that autocorrelation exists by using data from the same area at different prey abundance, as one of the fundamental rules of whether a species is captured and killed is the probability of coming in contact with the predator and this varies with prey density. The unpublished data come from two sites in South Africa. Shamwari Game Reserve (J. O’Brien, unpubl. data) covers 19 746 ha in the Eastern Cape Province where cheetah were reintroduced in 2000. The 55 000 ha Madikwe Game Reserve in the North West Province was gazetted in 1991 with a small population of cheetah extant (Hofmeyr et al., 2003). This population was supplemented from 1994 onwards and contains 19 individuals (Hofmeyr & van Dyk, 1998). Numerous studies provided excellent descriptive information on cheetah diet but insufficient information on prey abundance (Graham, 1966; McVittie, 1979; Stander, 1990; Purchase & du Toit, 2000; Cronje, Reilly & MacFadyen, 2002; Marker et al., 2003; Radloff & du Toit, 2004). Unless other sources could be found that provided prey abundance for the relevant time (Table 2), these studies could not be used in this analysis. The data collected in these studies were derived from both incidental observations and continuous follows. Continuous follows are widely regarded as the superior method of ascertaining the diet of a predator (Bertram, 1979; Mills, 1992); however, because of the relatively brief period cheetahs spend on kills, there is no difference between the two methods in measuring the diet of cheetahs (Mills, 1992; Hunter, 1998).

Data analysis 
Prey preference Jacobs’ index (Jacobs, 1974) minimizes the problems associated with many preference indices (non-linearity, bias to rare food items, increasing confidence intervals with increasing heterogeneity, being unbound or undefined, and lacking symmetry; Chesson, 1978; Strauss, 1979; Norbury & Sanson, 1992; Hayward & Kerley, 2005) and we chose this method. Jacobs’ index D ¼ r  p r þ p  2rp ð1Þ standardizes the relationship between prey relative abundance p (i.e. the proportion that each species makes up of the total abundance of censused prey species at a site) and the relative proportion that each species comprises as cheetah kills r to between +1 and 1, where +1 indicates maximum preference and 1 maximum avoidance. A Jacobs’ index value was calculated for each prey species at each site using prey abundance and kill data presented for that site in the literature (Tables 1 and 2). The mean Jacobs’ index of each prey species across studies was then calculated ( 1 SE wherever the mean is shown) and these values were tested for significant preference or avoidance using t-tests against a mean of 0 if they conformed to the assumptions of normality (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) (Palomares et al., 2001; Hayward & Kerley, 2005; Hayward, de Tores & Banks, 2005; Hayward et al., 2006a). Where transformation could not satisfy these assumptions, the sign test was used (Zar, 1996). If a species was killed more frequently than its relative abundance then it was considered preferred, whereas if a species was killed less frequently than its relative abundance it was considered avoided. Obviously, this is a simplification as this reflects not just the predator’s preference but also the prey’s vulnerability and the ease with which it is captured. This kind of analysis is not biased by the results from one particular area; is not overly influenced by the available community of prey species because for a species to be significantly preferred or avoided it must be so in diverse communities throughout its range; and it takes account of varying hunting group sizes and sex ratios by being collected in populations of males and females that hunt in various group sizes and compositions. Furthermore, variation in the effects of carnivore group size on components of predation cannot be made for a species living in the same habitat (Caro, 1994); therefore, there is no justification for teasing apart group sizes and sexes of hunters, particularly where little such information is given in the literature.

Table 2 Assumptions made in determining kills and prey abundance for studies where it is not implicitly stated

*This image is copyright of its original author

Multiple regression analysis 
Multiple regression was conducted on non-correlating, transformed variables to determine which factors influenced the prey preferences of the cheetah. Variables used were relative abundance of prey at a site (as an estimate of availability), prey body mass, herd size, preferred habitat type and threat of injury to the predator (Table 3). Categories were used for several of these variables, as has occurred previously (Funston, Mills & Biggs, 2001; Mills et al., 2004; Hayward & Kerley, 2005); however, although we readily acknowledge that this is a simplification of very complex issues, we believe the use of such categories serves to illustrate general trends. Cheetahs are generally thought to eat prey of medium body size (Schaller, 1968; Mills et al., 2004). Three-quarters of the mean adult female body mass of prey species was used in order to take account of calves and sub-adults eaten. This value was used in previous studies (Hayward & Kerley, 2005; Hayward, 2006; Hayward et al. 2006a,b) following Schaller’s (1972) example and we continue its use here. Weights were taken from Stuart & Stuart (2000). Social organization is an indicator of the ability of prey to detect predators and the ability of predators to get close to prey. Cheetahs have a higher hunting success on herds of less than 30 individuals (Eaton, 1974), perhaps because larger groups can detect cheetahs at greater distances (Fitzgibbon, 1990), suggesting that there may be a relationship between cheetah prey preference and herd size. The dilution effect also acts to reduce the probability that an individual in a large herd will be preyed upon (Hamilton, 1971; Dehn, 1990). Such benefits of herding are counteracted as larger herds are easier to detect and allow closer approach (Dehn, 1990). We used a categorical variable to investigate this issue (Table 3). Increased vegetation density reduces the detectability of prey and the speed of cheetah pursuit, and therefore habitat type may affect predation rates. Cheetahs occur in habitats ranging from desert to open grassland and woodland savanna (Eaton, 1970, 1974; Mills, 1998), suggesting that species preferring these habitats would be at greater risk of cheetah predation than those that rarely use them. Cheetahs also have a greater hunting success in more densely vegetated areas than the Serengeti short grass plains (Eaton, 1970), suggesting that prey in such areas are at even greater risk. Prey animals inhabiting dense vegetation generally adopt a silent, solitary, ‘hider’ strategy to evade detection, whereas prey on open grasslands are detected by sight rather than sound and can exist in large herds (Geist, 1974; Leuthold & Leuthold, 1975). A categorical variable of habitat density was used and, while this was a simplification, this technique has been used successfully for cheetah previously (Mills et al., 2004). The anti-predatory strategy a species uses affects its chances of becoming prey. Cryptic coloration and patterning in predators is an obvious way of improving hunting success; however, there has been no scientific comparison of crypsis between prey species or their predators, although inhabitants of dense vegetation are often cryptic or of dull body coloration compared with grassland species (Geist, 1974). Similarly there has been little work on the evasion speed of prey species (but see Elliott et al., 1977; Prins & Iason, 1989) and these issues meant the threat of injury to a hunter was all that could be analysed, where larger species are more likely to stand and fight predators than smaller ones (Geist, 1974) and an aggressive nature or dangerous weaponry are also factors. Again a categorical variable was used with information for each of these categories from Estes (1999) and Stuart & Stuart (2000).

Other analyses 
Significant relationships were plotted using distance weighted least-squares and linear regression fits of transformed data of the variables used in the multiple regression analysis. We also tested whether cheetahs were actually preying upon their preferred prey species using Spearman’s rank order correlation and whether there was a significant difference between preferred and avoided species for each predictor variable used in multiple regression using the Mann–Whitney test (Zar, 1996). Analysis of variance was used to test whether cheetahs in denser habitats preferred larger prey species due to the refuge from kleptoparasitism these habitats afforded. The habitat at several sites where Jacobs’ index values were calculated was classified according to their vegetation density (i.e. 1, Serengeti; 2, Kgalagadi; 3, Nairobi; 4, Kruger; 5, Timbavati and Klaserie; from Mills et al., 2004). Body mass was weighted by multiplying the body mass (threequarters of adult female mass) of the prey species by the proportion it comprises of the total prey killed at a site.

Results 
Jacobs’ index scores were calculated from 3909 kills of 58 species recorded as prey of the cheetah. Impala were killed by cheetah in 26 out of 27 studies where they were present, while blesbok (6 out of 6 studies), kudu (18/19), steenbok (7/8), common duiker, Thomson’s gazelle and springbok (8/9 for each) and hartebeest (17/19) were also commonly taken (Table 3; Fig. 1). Impala were also the most abundant species numerically (24.5% of available prey at 27 sites), followed by Thomson’s gazelle (23.1% at nine sites), blue wildebeest (16.0% at 26 sites), springbok (18.6% at nine sites) and plains zebra (10.5% at 28 sites) (Table 3). Thomson’s gazelle were the most frequently killed prey species (46.7% of kills from nine sites), followed by impala (32.0% at 27 sites), springbok (26.3% at nine sites) and Grant’s gazelle (13.2% at eight sites) (Table 3). Each of these four species were killed proportionally more frequently than they were available. Irrespectively, there was a significant positive relationship between the abundance of cheetah prey and the proportion with which it is killed (Spearman’s rank order correlation coefficient R= 0.840, n= 34, Po0.050), although there were no such relationships for individual species. Blesbuck (t= 8.61, d.f.= 5, Po0.001) and impala (Z= 26.9, n= 26, P= 0.031) were the only species that were significantly preferred by cheetah when all data were used. The exclusion of Kruuk & Turner’s (1967) data from the Serengeti led to Grant’s gazelle being significantly preferred (t= 5.66, d.f.= 6, P= 0.001). Similarly, excluding Foster & McLaughlin’s (1968) small sample from Nairobi National Park data led to Thomson’s gazelle being significantly preferred (t= 4.76, d.f.= 7, P= 0.002). The exclusion of one year’s data (2003) from the newly reintroduced cheetah population at Shamwari resulted in springbok (t=3.65, d.f.= 7, P= 0.008) being significantly preferred also. Justification for this is discussed below. An increased sample size for common reedbuck and common duiker may also result in them being significantly preferred if the existing trends continue (Fig. 2). Baboon (Z=100, n= 9, Po0.001), buffalo (Z= 100, n= 22, Po0.001), bushpig (Z= 100, n= 9, Po0.001), topi/tsessebe (Z= 91, n= 11, P= 0.016), eland (Z= 100, n= 20, Po0.001), elephant (Z= 100, n= 9, Po0.001), gemsbok (Z=90, n=10, P= 0.027), giraffe (Z=100, n= 22, Po0.001), hippopotamus (Z= 100, n= 6, P= 0.031), ostrich (Z= 81, n= 16, P= 0.024), rhinoceros (black and white combined; Z= 100, n= 13, Po0.001), sable (t=3.40, d.f.= 6, P= 0.014), warthog (Z= 81, n= 26, P= 0.003), blue wildebeest (t=9.40, d.f.= 25, Po0.001) and plains zebra (Z= 93, n= 28, Po0.001) were all significantly avoided (Fig. 2). Bontebok, black wildebeest, blue duiker, bushbuck, hartebeest, klipspringer, kudu, lechwe, nyala, oribi, porcupine, roan, Cape and Sharpe’s grysbok, steenbok, waterbuck, common and mountain reedbuck, monkey, mountain zebra and bat-eared fox were all taken in accordance with their abundance (Fig. 2). A larger sample size, however, may lead to hartebeest, nyala, bat-eared fox, mountain reedbuck, roan, Sharpe’s and Cape grysbok, oribi, bontebok, blue duiker, klipspringer, lechwe, porcupine, vervet monkey, black wildebeest, mountain zebra and several carnivore species being avoided if the existing trends continue (Fig. 2). A multiple linear regression analysis was performed on species with more than two Jacobs’ index estimates using prey abundance, body mass and habitat type variables (Table 3) after increased prey abundance was found to correlate with increased herd size (Spearman’s rank order correlation coefficient R= 0.37, n= 36, Po0.05) and decreased threat (R=0.37, n= 36, Po0.05). The Jacobs’ index value of a species was predicted by the equation Jacobs’ index= 1.09+0.45(log(abundance))–0.23(log(body mass))–0.25(habitat) (R2 = 0.458, F3,32= 9.001, Po0.001). Proportional prey abundance and body mass were significant predictive variables (Po0.001; Table 4; Fig. 3). When these five potential predictive variables are viewed independently, only the threat of injury to the hunting cheetah differs significantly between preferred and avoided prey species (Mann–Whitney U= 2.93, n= 30, P= 0.002; Fig. 4). A more detailed look at the relationship between Jacobs’ index and prey body mass (i.e. for body masses less than 200 kg) using distance-weighted least-squares (DWLS) regression shows that the most preferred prey of cheetah weigh between 23 and 56 kg with an ideal weight range (mode) of 36 kg (DWLS r= 0.350, n= 30, P=0.058; Fig. 5). There is another rise in the plot of Jacobs’ index values at 120 kg (Fig. 5). This distribution is reflected in the range of prey body mass cheetahs actually take, and there is a significant relationship between what cheetahs prefer and what they actually kill (Spearman’s rank order correlation coefficient R= 0.891, n= 30, Po0.05) despite the maximum of the prey killed plot occurring somewhat lower than that preferred at 25 kg (Fig. 5). The mean body mass of the preferred prey species (i.e. blesbok, impala, Grant’s and Thomson’s gazelles, and springbok) was 27.3 4.8 kg. If we take 30 kg as threequarters of the adult female body mass of the cheetah (Stuart & Stuart, 2000), then the ratio of predator to preferred prey is 1:1.2 based on the ideal body mass of prey or 1:0.9 based on the mean body mass of preferred prey. There was no significant difference between the weighted body mass of prey species in different habitat densities (Fig. 6). Thus there is no preference for larger prey items in more densely vegetated areas that offer refuge from kleptoparasitism.

Figure 1 Commonly killed and avoided prey of cheetah Acinonyx jubatus.

*This image is copyright of its original author


Figure 2 Cheetah Acinonyx jubatus dietary preferences (mean Jacobs’ index 1 SE) from 22 populations at differing prey densities. Black bars represent species taken significantly more frequently than expected based on their abundance (preferred), grey bars indicate species taken in accordance with their relative abundance and unfilled bars show species killed significantly less frequently than expected based on their abundance (avoided). Only species with more than two estimates of Jacobs’ index are included and ‘Excl’. after a species name indicates the Jacobs’ index value when outlying sites were excluded.

*This image is copyright of its original author

Discussion 
Cheetahs prefer abundant prey within a range of 23–56 kg and that have a mean mass of 27 kg, and actually take prey of similar, although slightly smaller, dimensions (Fig. 5). Prey inhabiting open grasslands seem particularly susceptible to cheetah predation (Fig. 4), suggesting that cheetahs are highly adapted to this niche as a predator of gazelles and the larger blesbok. The once great migrations that crossed the southern African highveld grasslands with springbok and blesbok herds numbering millions (Skead, 1987) may have been the ideal environment for the cheetah, depending upon the density of larger competitors. Similarly, the decline of the Thomson’s gazelle population in the Serengeti in the 1970s and 1980s (Borner et al., 1987; Ottichilo et al., 2000; but questioned by Dublin et al., 1990) may have caused the decline in cheetah reproductive success observed over that period (Kelly et al., 1998) and led to a decline in cheetah abundance there. The preferred prey species offer minimal injury risk (Fig. 4) and their small size means cheetahs can bolt down their meat before kleptoparasites arrive (as reflected by the speed with which they eat: Mills, 1992; Hunter, 1998), without risking losing too much food. This is probably an artefact of the morphological limitations of cheetahs to the size of prey they can capture given that cheetahs in denser habitats that afford refuge from, and lower levels of, kleptoparasitism do not prefer larger prey than those in more open areas (Fig. 6). Other morphological features, such as the wide nares, may also be adaptations to assist rapid feeding. As no study we used lacked dominant competitors of the cheetah, we were unable to test directly whether cheetah select larger prey in the absence of lions or spotted hyaenas, as occurs in Namibian rangelands (McVittie, 1979). Given the apparent selective forces acting on the cheetah from these dominant competitors (Laurenson, 1995; Durant, 1998, 2000a), there seems no impetus for cheetahs to kill prey larger than they can consume before kleptoparasites arrive. All these features highlight the importance of optimality (Pyke et al., 1977; Krebs, 1978) to cheetah foraging. The small body size, semi-retractile claws and gracile limbs of the cheetah impose morphological limitations on the size of prey that can be captured (energetic benefit), with ease and with minimal risk of injury (energetic costs). Kleptoparasitism imposes another cost that cheetahs must consider when foraging, and this explains their avoidance of lions and spotted hyaenas (Durant, 2000a,b). These species are also predation threats to cheetah, which imposes another limitation to optimality (Krebs, 1980). Although regular opportunistic predation acts to cloud evidence of optimality, this in itself is further substantiation that decisions made by the predator about the energy obtainable from a prey item after consideration of the energy expended during the hunt, the likelihood of success and the risk of injury, act to direct the predator to hunt or not. Unfortunately, we cannot test whether decisions made to optimize the net energetic benefits of a hunt result in increased reproductive fitness, and thereby suffer the limitations of all studies based on the premise of optimal foraging (Pierce & Ollason, 1987). The causes of cheetah prey preferences stand in stark contrast to those of the lion (Hayward & Kerley, 2005). Where the cheetah preferentially preys upon medium-sized prey (23–56 kg), the lion prefers much larger prey species (190–550 kg). Where cheetah select prey based on its abundance, lions select prey based on its body mass. Given the differences in body mass between the two species, niche theory suggests that they should segregate the dietary niche available to them in Africa (Hutchinson, 1959; Wilson, 1975a); however, there is substantial overlap in their diets (Table 3; Hayward & Kerley, 2005). Rather, these two predators prefer to prey upon different species (based on the significantly preferred prey unique to each), but opportunistic predation acts to create dietary overlap. The preferred weight range of cheetahs (23–56 kg) encompasses 15 prey species, but only five are significantly preferred. These are blesbok, impala, springbok, and Thomson’s and Grant’s gazelles (Fig. 1). Common and bohor reedbuck, and common duiker are also largely within this weight range and all regularly inhabit areas where cheetah predominantly hunt (Mills et al., 2004). Increased sample size is also likely to see these species significantly preferred if the current trends continue (Fig. 2), and other data support this as cheetah predation led to a precipitous decline in reedbuck numbers in Phinda (Hunter, 1998, p. 149). As Thomson’s gazelles are below the preferred weight range (based on three-quarters of adult female body mass), it is not surprising that cheetahs take adult male Thomson’s gazelles most frequently (Fitzgibbon & Fanshawe, 1989) Thomson’s gazelle is taken preferentially on the Serengeti plains over impala and Grant’s gazelle (Kruuk & Turner, 1967; Schaller, 1968), but in Nairobi Grant’s gazelle and impala are preferred over Thomson’s gazelle (Foster & McLaughlin, 1968). The numerical dominance of Thomson’s gazelle in the Serengeti indicates that it may be uneconomical to hunt anything else (Eaton, 1974), and the higher hunting success rate in the Serengeti woodlands (Eaton, 1970) reinforces that Thomson’s gazelles may be almost all that can be captured in open areas, despite being sub-optimal (Fig. 5). This is particularly the case during the breeding season when Thomson’s gazelle fawns are captured in almost every hunting attempt (Schaller, 1968). The preference for larger impala and Grant’s gazelle in denser vegetation suggests that increased habitat density (as found in Nairobi compared with the Serengeti plains) affords cheetah stalking cover that allows them to capture larger, more profitable, prey items, but this is likely to reduce the maximum speed cheetahs and their prey can reach in dense vegetation. As preferred prey of cheetahs, Thomson’s gazelles are likely to avoid them and point scans show that gazelles are in fact less common in the vicinity of cheetahs than they are in the vicinity of lions P. leo and spotted hyaenas C. crocuta (Durant, 1998), both of which significantly avoid gazelles as prey (Hayward & Kerley, 2005; Hayward, 2006; Hayward et al., 2006b). Therefore, whether cheetahs occur in areas supporting a low density of Thomson’s gazelle through avoidance of encounter competition (as suggested by Creel, Spong & Creel, 2001) or through the localized predator avoidance movements of the gazelle away from their most serious predator is unknown. Finally from a conservation viewpoint, strategies to increase the density of cheetah, and other threatened predators, can now be made with a knowledge of what prey species need to increase in abundance to see a corresponding increase in a specific predator. Such a strategy has been suggested for tiger Panthera tigris and leopard P. pardus in India (Ramakrishnan, Coss & Pelkey, 1999) and wolves Canis lupus in Europe (Meriggi & Lovari, 1996); however, the results here provide detailed information as to what species are required for cheetah. Our results will also prove useful for conservation managers in India who are considering reintroducing cheetah (Marker, 1998) by illustrating the body mass of prey that are likely to be preferentially killed, and therefore required, by cheetah. Similarly, research into the remnant cheetah population in Iran may also benefit from this information.

Table 4 Regression statistics for the multiple regression model Jacobs’ index=1.088+0.451(log(abundance))0.225(log(body mass)) 0.252 habitat: using prey species with more than two estimates of Jacobs’ index and excluding carnivores

*This image is copyright of its original author



*This image is copyright of its original author


*This image is copyright of its original author


*This image is copyright of its original author


*This image is copyright of its original author


Note: I wasn't able to attach some of the tables, if you want to read the data in those tables click on the link at the top of this post.
Reply

Bangladesh TheHyenid76 Offline
Regular Member
***
#24

Coalitions of male cheetahs are powerful predators. Adult zebra have been killed by cheetah coalitions. Photo Source


*This image is copyright of its original author




1 user Likes TheHyenid76's post
Reply






Users browsing this thread:
5 Guest(s)

About Us
Go Social     Subscribe  

Welcome to WILDFACT forum, a website that focuses on sharing the joy that wildlife has on offer. We welcome all wildlife lovers to join us in sharing that joy. As a member you can share your research, knowledge and experience on animals with the community.
wildfact.com is intended to serve as an online resource for wildlife lovers of all skill levels from beginners to professionals and from all fields that belong to wildlife anyhow. Our focus area is wild animals from all over world. Content generated here will help showcase the work of wildlife experts and lovers to the world. We believe by the help of your informative article and content we will succeed to educate the world, how these beautiful animals are important to survival of all man kind.
Many thanks for visiting wildfact.com. We hope you will keep visiting wildfact regularly and will refer other members who have passion for wildlife.

Forum software by © MyBB